<< Back To Home

WHAT'S NEW THIS TUESDAY: EARLY LIFE HEALTH AND ADULT CIRCUMSTANCE IN DEVELOPING COUNTRIES

Sunday, 30th of September 2012 Print

 'A growing literature shows that events in early life have long-term consequences for adult health, cognition and labor market success, both in developed and in developing countries. . . . 100 percent eradication of malarial infections increased subsequent adult income by 47 percent in the US, 45 percent in Brazil, 45 percent in Colombia, and 41 percent in Mexico.'

 

  • EARLY-LIFE HEALTH AND ADULT CIRCUMSTANCE IN DEVELOPING COUNTRIES 

 

NBER WORKING PAPER SERIES

  

Janet Currie

Tom Vogl

Working Paper 18371

 

Text  excerpts below; best viewed, with equations and annexes, at http://www.nber.org/papers/w18371.pdf

 

NATIONAL BUREAU OF ECONOMIC RESEARCH

1050 Massachusetts Avenue

Cambridge, MA 02138

September 2012

 

We would like to thank Anne Case for helpful conversations and Diane Alexander for excellent research assistance. The views expressed herein are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Bureau of Economic Research.

 

NBER working papers are circulated for discussion and comment purposes. They have not been peer reviewed or been subject to the review by the NBER Board of Directors that accompanies official NBER publications.

 

© 2012 by Janet Currie and Tom Vogl. All rights reserved. Short sections of text, not to exceed two paragraphs, may be quoted without explicit permission provided that full credit, including © notice,is given to the source.

 

Early-Life Health and Adult Circumstance in Developing Countries

Janet Currie and Tom Vogl

NBER Working Paper No. 18371

September 2012

JEL No. I12,I15,J24,O15

 

ABSTRACT

A growing literature documents the links between long-term outcomes and health in the fetal period, infancy, and early childhood. Much of this literature focuses on rich countries, but researchers are increasingly taking advantage of new sources of data and identification to study the long reach of childhood health in developing countries. Health in early life may be a more significant determinant of adult outcomes in these countries because health insults are more frequent, the capacity to remediate is more limited, and multiple shocks may interact. However, the underlying relationships may also be more difficult to measure, given significant mortality selection. We survey recent evidence on the adult correlates of early-life health and the long-term effects of shocks due to disease, famine, malnutrition, pollution, and war.

 

Janet Currie

Princeton University

316 Wallace Hall

Princeton, NJ 08544

and NBER

jcurrie@princeton.edu

Tom Vogl

Department of Economics

Princeton University

363 Wallace Hall

Princeton, NJ 08544

and NBER

tvogl@princeton.edu

 

1. Introduction

 

A growing body of evidence documents that healthy children tend to become healthy and wealthy adults (Almond and Currie 2010, 2011; Currie 2011). For example, anthropometric markers such as birth weight and child height are related to future schooling, employment, earnings, family formation, and health. Findings from natural experiments suggest that these relationships in large part reflect a pathway running from childhood health to adult outcomes.

 

Tothe extent that inequalities and shortfalls in adult outcomes are predicted by early-life health,these results may offer additional justification for policies aimed at improving child health.

We argue that the long-term effects of such shocks are likely to be larger in developing countries for several reasons. First, such shocks are simply more frequent in many developing countries than in the industrialized world, suggesting that the lingering effects of early-life health problems may well be more important in developing countries. Second, child health shocks are likely to interact. For example, a child who is malnourished may be less able to ward off, or to recover from, disease. Similarly, a mother who was malnourished may bear a child who is compromised in her ability to cope with health insults. Moreover, if there are non-linearities in the production function for child health, then the same shock may have quite different effects depending on starting levels of health.

 . . .

 

Changes in Average Height

The literature on height is in part motivated by the remarkable increasing secular trend in heights over the past two centuries. The trend began in Europe, which economic historians attribute to rising living standards (Floud, Wachter, and Gregory 1990; Fogel 2004; Steckel 1995, 2008).8 Japan followed somewhat later (Shay 1994), as the Japanese economy entered a period of rapid growth in the late nineteenth century. Many countries still classified today as “developing” also saw rising average heights across cohorts born in the twentieth century (Strauss and Thomas 1998). These transformations in the average size of the human body occurred too quickly to be driven by evolution.

 

Indeed, researchers have now amassed considerable evidence that these secular increases in height have antecedents in early-life health. Many studies show that child and adult height are associated with parental socioeconomic status, nutrition, and access to clean water and sanitation during childhood.9 Additionally, a number of the quasi-experimental studies reviewed below suggest that these associations represent .causal effects of childhood conditions.

 

. . . within Africa, where mortality rates are high, declines in child mortality are associated with decreased height, and national income bears no relation to height after controlling for mortality. These findings suggest that selective mortality may be an important force in determining the distribution of heights among African adults. In this sense, they offer one explanation for why Africans are much taller than one would expect in light of their incomes.

. . .

 

3. Evidence Regarding the Longer Term Effects of Early-Life Shocks

 

. . .

 

Nutrition

 

Table 3 provides an overview of some recent studies examining the longer-term impacts of poor nutrition in childhood. Here we focus on moderate and chronic malnutrition; we discuss famine in the next section. Recent studies have focused on the roles of specific nutrients, and on the question of whether children recover from nutritional shocks during critical periods, such as while they are in utero or in the first year of life.

 

In one recent study, Malluccio et al. (2009) report on a long-running randomized INCAP (Institute of Nutrition of Central America and Panama) experiment that involved giving Guatemalan children zero to seven years old a high-protein energy drink (the treatment) or a placebo with a similar number of calories. The findings suggest that the treatment increased schooling attainment among women by 1.2 grades, and cognitive ability by .24 standard deviations for both men and women. Hoddinott et al. (2008) study the same intervention and find that nutritional supplements in the first two years of life increased male wages by 46 percent.13 These large effects suggest that an early diet that is sufficient in calories but not in protein can have severe long-term consequences.

 

. . . cohorts affected by maternal fasting are 22 to 23 percent more likely to be disabled in adulthood. In comparison, the estimated effect on disability rates among Arab-named adults in the U.S. is only slightly lower, at 19 percent.

  

Famine

 

Several attributes distinguish famines from the localized rainfall shocks that Maccini and Yang (2009) and Pathania (2009) study. First, in general equilibrium, famines affect both food prices and farmers’ incomes, while localized rainfall shortfalls affect only incomes. Second, perhaps due in part to price adjustments, famines are typically more severe than localized shocks, with massive effects on mortality. As a result, concerns about selective mortality are likely to be more relevant in studies of famine. Third, unlike localized weather shocks, famines arguably have as much to do with politics as with food output (Drèze and Sen 1991). Thus, research strategies designed to measure the effects of localized rainfall shocks may fail to generate plausible estimates of the effects of famine.

 

. . .

 

The Chinese famine has received particular attention, and several papers provide evidence complementary to Meng and Qian’s. As with the studies described in the previous paragraph, analyses of the Chinese famine use both pre- and post-famine cohorts as control groups.
 

Chen and Zhou (2007) examine the effects of the Chinese famine on height, finding a decrease of 3 cm for those born or conceived during the famine. Gørgens et al. (2012) report a similar stunting effect of the famine on the rural Chinese population for whom the famine was most severe. In further analyses of socioeconomic outcomes, Almond et al. (2010) estimate that women (men) exposed to the Chinese famine in utero were 3 percent (5.9 percent) less likely to work and 13 percent (12 percent) more likely to be disabled, and that men were 6.5 percent less likely to be married. Finally, building on earlier research examining the effects of the Dutch “hunger winter” of 1944 on a wide range of outcomes (see for example, Lumey et al. 2011), two papers investigate the effects of the 1959 Chinese famine on other health outcomes. Fung (2009) demonstrates that in utero exposure to famine predicts obesity for women, but not for men, while St. Clair et al. (2005) find that prenatal and infant exposure to famine doubles the likelihood of developing schizophrenia.

 

Since famine often involves substantial mortality, it can be difficult to interpret the magnitudes of the effects. Indeed, the data suggest that mortality selection may severely dampen the estimated effects of early-life shocks on the average height of adult survivors. For example, data on Chinese adults show little sign of a decline in average heights among cohorts exposed to the Great Famine in early life unless one accounts for selection (Gørgens et al. 2012). In Meng and Qian’s study (2009) stunting effects are most noticeable at the upper quantiles of the height distribution, as would be expected if mortality culled individuals from the bottom tail of the height distribution.

 

Disease

 

. . .

 

Three papers apply this method in asking whether early-life malaria exposure affects socioeconomic outcomes in adulthood. Using data from the U.S. South and Latin America, Bleakley (2010) estimates that 100 percent eradication of malarial infections increased subsequent adult income by 47 percent in the US, 45 percent in Brazil, 45 percent in Colombia, and 41 percent in Mexico. Bleakley also finds positive effects on literacy, but mixed results for years of schooling. Cutler et al. (2010), studying India’s malaria eradication campaign of the 1950s, also detect no effect on educational attainment. However, consistent with Bleakley (2010), they find that malaria eradication increased household consumption, perhaps implying that the eradication of malaria increases incomes by allowing people to work more, or at higher productivity jobs.

 

. . .

 

Complementing Bleakley’s (2007) work on the historical United States, several studies examine the effect of deworming on medium and long-term outcomes in developing countries. In a well-known paper, Miguel and Kremer (2004) analyze the experimental introduction of deworming treatments in Kenyan schools. The deworming intervention increased school attendance by 7 percent but had no effect on test scores. In a follow-up study, however, Baird et al. (2011) find that self-reported health, years of schooling, and test scores all increased for the treated group. Hours worked rose 12 percent in the full sample and 20 percent among those who were working for wages. Moreover, Ozier (2011) shows that deworming school-aged children had positive externalities on very young children in the same community, fitting for a communicable disease like helminth infection. Children less than one year old during the treatment experienced IQ gains on the order of 0.2-standard deviations, which translated into an additional 0.5-0.8 years of schooling.

 

Pollution

 

. . .

 Brainerd and Menon (2011) find that a 10 percent increase in chemical runoff from fertilizers in the water increases infant mortality by11.26 percent and decreases height for age by 0.14 standard deviations.

 

There is now considerable evidence from developed countries that air pollution has harmful effects on birth weight and infant mortality (see Currie, 2011 for a recent summary of this literature) and a growing literature showing that this is also true in the developed world. Foster et al. (2009) study air pollution in Mexico using satellite measures of aerosol optical depth. Variation in pollution comes from a voluntary certification program, and regional variation in the supply of auditors available to implement the program. They find that a 3.6 percent improvement in aerosol optical depth resulted in a 16 percent decline in infant mortality due to respiratory illness, but no decline from deaths due to external causes (a control cause). Jayachandran (2009) estimates OLS models where an average aerosol index (TOMS) captures the variation in pollution levels that resulted from the 1997 Indonesian wildfires. Her results indicate that areas affected by the smoke suffered a 1.2 percent reduction in birth cohort size with the largest effects among the poor, a result that highlights the importance of selection once again. Despite these concerns about selection bias, the Wang et al. study mentioned above found that each 100-ug/m3 increase in SO2 (TSP) exposure in the third trimester reduces birth weight by 7.3 (6.9) grams and increased the probability of low birth weight by 11 percent (10 percent).

 

War

 

. . .

 

A growing literature, reviewed in Table 6, suggests that exposure in early childhood may also have significant long-term scarring effects. Aguero and Deolalikar (2012) estimate the effect of childhood exposure to the Rwandan genocide on women’s height-for-age z-scores. Using difference-in-differences for exposed cohorts with Zimbabwean women as the control group, they find that Rwandan women exposed to the conflict have 0.2-standard deviations lower height-for-age z-scores. Taking Akresh and Verwimp (2006) and Aguero and Deolalikar (2012) together, it appears that the Rwandan civil war greatly affected the height of girls in Rwanda, and that even exposure late in adolescence affected adult female height.

 

Several papers evaluate the effects of conflict on years of schooling. Alderman et al. (2006) combine maternal fixed effects and IV estimation to show that exposure to civil war and drought shocks in early childhood in Zimbabwe reduced children’s height-for-age z-scores, which subsequently had negative effects on years of schooling and adult height. Akresh and de Walque (2008) estimate a difference-in-difference model and find that children exposed to the Rwandan genocide earned 0.4 fewer years of schooling. In Peru, León (2010) estimates that for each additional year of civil war exposure in early childhood, children acquired 0.07 fewer years of schooling. Therefore, a child exposed for the first three years of life attains, on average, 0.21 fewer years of schooling. Some of this effect on education may work through the destruction of school buildings.

  

4. Discussion and Conclusions

. . .

 

Case and Paxson (2011) make the further important point that the societal response to disease can impact long-term child outcomes through its effects on health infrastructure. Using data from fourteen sub-Saharan African countries, they find that in countries heavily impacted by HIV, skilled birth attendance, antenatal care, and immunization rates declined between 1988 and 2005. Countries with a minimal burden from HIV experienced no such declines. This deterioration, which presumably occurred because of a diversion of health resources to fight the AIDS epidemic, may have lasting effects on children even if neither they nor their parents are HIVpositive.

 

In sum, research in the past decade has shown that health shocks in early-life have longterm effects on adults in both the developed and developing world. But the literature still has little understanding of heterogeneity in these effects. Future research should focus on identifying pathways and mechanisms; measuring the relative magnitudes of the effects of different health shocks; examining interactions between shocks; and revisiting the question of critical periods.

 

Acknowledgements

 

We would like to thank Anne Case for helpful conversations and Diane Alexander for excellent research assistance.

 

References

 

1. Abbott RD, White LR, Webster RG, Petrovitch H, Masaki KH, Snowdon DA, Curb JD.

1998. Height as a Marker of Childhood Development and Late-Life Cognitive Function:

The Honolulu-Asia Aging Study. Pediatrics 102(3):602-9

2. Adair LS, Guilkey DK. 1997. Age-specific determinants of stunting in Filipino children. J.

Nutr. 127(2):314-20

3. Adair LS. 2007. Size at birth and growth trajectories to young adulthood. Am. J. Hum. Biol.19:327–37

4. Aguero JM, Deolalikar A. 2012. Late Bloomers? Identifying Critical Periods in Human

Capital Accumulation. Evidence from the Rwanda Genocide. Presented at 9th Midwest Int.

Econ. Devel. Conf., Univ. of Minn.

5. Akresh R, de Walque D. 2008. Armed Conflict and Schooling: Evidence from the 1994

Rwandan Genocide. World Bank Policy Research Work Pap. #4606.

6. Akresh R, Bhalotra S, Marinella L, Osili U. 2011. War and Stature: Growing Up During

the Nigerian Civil War. IZA Discussion Paper No. 6194.

7. Akresh R, Lucchetti L, Thirumurthy H. 2013. Wars and Child Health: Evidence from the

Eritrean-Ethiopian Conflict. J. Dev. Econ., in press.

8. Akresh R, Verwimp P, Bundervoet T. 2007. Civil War, Crop Failure, and Child Stunting in

Rwanda. Tech. Rep. World Bank Policy Research WP 4208.

9. Alderman H, Hoddinott J, Kinsey B. 2006. Long-Term Consequences of Early Childhood

Malnutrition. Oxf. Econ. Pap. 58(3):450-74

10. Almond D. 2006. Is the 1918 Influenza Pandemic Over? Long-Term Effects of in Utero

Influenza Exposure in the Post-1940 U.S. Population. J. Polit. Econ. 114:672-712

11. Almond D, Currie J. 2011. Killing Me Softly: The Fetal Origins Hypothesis. J. Econ.

Perspect. 25(3): 153-72

12. Almond D, Currie J. 2010. Human Capital Development before Age Five. In The Handbook

of Labor Economics, 4b. ed. O Ashenfelter, D Card, 15:1315-486. Amsterdam: Elsevier

Science B.V.

13. Almond D, Edlund L, Li H, Zhang J. 2010. Long-term Effects of the 1959-1961 China

Famine: Mainland China and Hong Kong. In The Economic Consequences of Demographic

Change in East Asia, University of Chicago Press. pp. 321-50.

14. Almond D, Mazumder B. 2011. Health Capital and the Prenatal Environment: The Effect

of Ramadan Observance during Pregnancy. Am. Econ. J.:Appl. Econ. 3(4):56-85

15. Barker DJP. 2001. Fetal Origins of Cardiovascular and Lung Disease. New York: Marcel Dekker, Inc.

16. Baird S, Hicks JH, Kremer M, Miguel E. 2011. Worms at Work: Long-run Impacts of Child

Health Gains. http://www.cgdev.org/doc/events/KLPS-Labor_2011-05-16-Circulate.pdf

17. Behrman JR, Hoddinott J, Maluccio JA, Martorell R. 2009. Brains versus Brawn: Labor

Market Returns to Intellectual and Health Human Capital in a Poor Developing Country.

Tech. Rep. Middlebury WP 0907.

18. Behrman JR, Calderon MC, Preston SH, Hoddinott J, Martorell R, Stein AD. 2009. Nutritional

supplementation in girls influences the growth of their children: prospective study in

Guatemala. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 90(5):1372-79

19. Behrman JR, Rosenzweig MR. 2005. The returns to birth weight. Rev. Econ. Stat. 86:586-

601

20. Bhalotra S, Rawlings S. 2011. Intergenerational Persistence in Health in Developing Countries:

The Penalty of Gender Inequality. J. Pub. Econ. 95(4):286-99

21. Bhalotra S, Rawlings S. 2013. Gradients of Intergenerational Transmission of Health in

Developing Countries. Rev. Econ. Stat., in press.

22. Bhalotra S, Venkataramani A. 2011. Is the Captain of the Men of Death Still At Play?

Long-Run Impacts of Early Life Pneumonia Exposure and the Sulfa Drug Revolution.

Work. Pap. Presented at NBER Children’s Meet. May 6, 2011.

23. Bharadwaj P, Eberhard J, Neilson C. 2010. Do Initial Endowments Matter Only Initially?

Birth Weight, Parental Investments and Academic Achievement in School. Tech. Rep.,

UCSD WP 9-16-2010.

24. Black SE, Devereux PJ, Salvanes KG. 2007. From the Cradle to the Labor Market? The

Effect of Birth Weight on Adult Outcomes. Q. J. Econ. 122(1):409-39

25. Blattman C, Annan J. 2010. The Consequences of Child Soldiering. Rev. Econ. Stats.

92(4):882-98

26. Bleakley H. 2010. Malaria Eradication in the Americas: A Retrospective Analysis of

Childhood Exposure. Am. Econ. J.: Appl. Econ. 2:1-45

27. Bozzoli C, Deaton A, Quintana-Domeque C. 2009. Adult Height and Childhood Disease.

Demography. 46(4):647-69

28. Brainerd E, Menon N. 2011. Seasonal Effects of Water Quality on Infant and Child Health in India. Presented at the 2nd IGC-ISI India Devel. Pol. Conf., Indian Statistical Institute, Delhi Centre.

29. Buckles K, Hungerman D. 2013. Season of Birth and Later Outcomes: Old Questions, New Answers.” Re. Stat. In press.

30. Bundervoet T, Verwimp P, Akresh R. 2009. Health and Civil War in Rural Burundi. J.

Hum. Resour. 44(2):536-63

31. Camacho A. 2008. Stress and Birth Weight: Evidence from Terrorist Attacks. Am. Econ.

Rev.: Pap. Proc. 98(2):511-15

32. Case A, Ardington C. 2006. The Impact of Parental Death on School Outcomes: Longitudinal Evidence from South Africa,” Demog. 43(3):401-420.

33. Case A, Paxson C. 2008. Stature and Status: Height, Ability, and Labor Market Outcomes. J. Polit. Econ. 116(3):499-532.

34. Case A, Paxson C, Ableidinger J. 2004. Orphans in Africa: Parental Death, Poverty and

School Enrollment,” Demog. 41(3):483-508.

35. Chay KY, Greenstone M. 2003. The Impact of Air Pollution on Infant Mortality: Evidence from Geographic Variation in Pollution Shocks Induced by a Recession. Q. J. Econ.118(3):1121-67

36. Chay KY, Greenstone M. 2003. Air Quality, Infant Mortality, and the Clean Air Act of

1970. NBER Work. Pap. No. 10053.

37. Checkley W, Gilman RH, Black RE, Epstein LD, Cabrera L, Sterling CR, Moulton LH.

2004. Effect of water and sanitation on childhood health in a poor Peruvian peri-urban

community. Lancet 363(9403):112-18

38. Chen Y, Zhou L. 2007. The Long-Term Health and Economic Consequences of the 1959-

1961 Famine in China. J Health Econ. 26:659-81

39. Costa D, Steckel R. 1997. Long-term trends in health, welfare, and economic growth in the United States. In Health and Welfare during Industrialization, ed. R Floud, R Steckel, 2.

Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

40. Currie J. 2009. Healthy, Wealthy, and Wise: Socioeconomic Status, Poor Health in Childhood, and Human Capital Development. J. Econ. Lit., Am. Econ. Assoc., 47(1):87-122.

41. Currie J. 2011. Inequality at Birth: Some Causes and Consequences. Am. Econ. Rev.,

Presented at the Annu. Richard T. Ely Lecture at the AEA meet. in Denver, CO.

42. Cutler D, Fung W, Kremer M, Singhal M, Vogl T. 2010. Early-life Malaria Exposure and

Adult Outcomes: Evidence from Malaria Eradication in India. Am. Econ. J.: Appl. Econ.

2:72-94

43. Deaton A. 2007. Height, Health and Development. P. Natl. Acad. of Sci. USA,

104(33):13232-37

44. Dercon S, Porter C. 2010. Live aid revisited: long-term impacts of the 1984 Ethiopian

famine on children. Tech. Rep., CSAE WPS 2010-39.

45. Evans D, Miguel E. 2007 Orphans and Schooling in Africa: Parental Death, Poverty and

School Enrollment. Demog. 44(1):35-57.

46. Ferrie J, Rolf K, Troesken W. 2011. Cognitive Disparities, Lead Plumbing, and Water

Chemistry: Intelligence Test Scores and Exposure to Water-Borne Lead among World War

Two U.S. Army Enlistees. NBER Work. Pap. No. 17161.

47. Field E, Glennerster R, Hussam R. 2011. Throwing the Baby out with the Drinking Water: Unintended Consequences of Arsenic Mitigation Efforts in Bangladesh. Wok. Pap. Dep. Econ., Harvard Univ.

48. Field E, Robles O, Torero M. 2009. Iodine Deficiency and Schooling Attainment in Tanzania. Am. Econ. J.:Appl. Econ. 1(4):140-69

49. Floud R, Wachter KW, Gregory AS. 1990. Height, health, and history: Nutritional status

in the United Kingdom, 1750-1980. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

50. Fogel RW. 2004. The Escape from Hunger and Premature Death, 1700–2100. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

51. Foster A, Gutierrez E, Kumar N. 2009. Voluntary Compliance, Pollution Levels, and Infant Mortality in Mexico. Am. Econ. Rev.:Pap. & Proc. 99(2):191-97

52. Fung, Winnie. 2009. Early Childhood Malnutrition and Adult Obesity: Evidence from the

1959-61 China Famine.

53. Fung, W. and Ha, W. 2010. "Intergenerational Effects of the 1959-61 China Famine." In

Fuentes-Nieva and Seck (eds.), Risks, Shocks, and Human Development: On the Brink,

Palgrave Macmillan, p.222-254.

54. Gorgens T, Xin M, Vaithianathan R. 2012. Stunting and selection effects of famine: A case study of the Great Chinese Famine. J. Dev. Econ. 97(1):99-111

55. Gowin EB. 1915. The Executive and His Control of Men. New York: Macmillan.

56. Haeffner LS, Barbieri MA, Rona RJ, Bettiol H, Silva AA. 2002. The relative strength of

weight and length at birth in contrast to social factors as determinants of height at 18 years

in Brazil. Ann. Hum. Biol. 29:627-40

57. Haddad LJ, Bouis HE. 1991. The Impact of Nutritional Status on Agricultural Productivity: Wage Evidence from the Philippines. Oxford B. Econ. Stat. 53:45-68

58. Hanna R, Duflo E, Greenstone M. 2012. Up in Smoke: The Influence of Household Behavior on the Long-Run Impact of Improved Cooking Stoves. Tech. Rep., MIT Dept. Econ. Work. Pap. No. 12-10.

59. Hensley WE. 1993. Height as a Measure of Success in Academe. Psychol.: J. Hum. Behav 30: 40-46

60. Hoddinott J, Maluccio JA, Behrman JR, Flores R, Martorell R. 2008. Effect of a nutrition

intervention during early childhood on economic productivity in Guatemalan adults. Lancet

371(9610):411-16

61. Huang W, Lei X, Ridder G, Strauss J, Zhao Y. 2012. Health, Height, Height Shrinkage and SES at Older Ages: Evidence from China. Tech. Rep., IZA DP No. 6489.

62. Jablonka E, Lamb MJ. 2005. Evolution in Four Dimensions: Genetic, Epigenetic, Behavioral, and Symbolic Variation in the History of Life. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.

63. Jayachandran S. 2009. Air Quality and Early-Life Mortality Evidence from Indonesia's

Wildfires. J. Hum. Resour. 44(4):916-54

64. Kelly E. 2009. The Scourge of Asian Flu: In utero Exposure to Pandemic Influenza and the Development of a Cohort of British Children. Institute for Fiscal Studies Work. Pap. No.

W09/17.

65. Klein RE, HE Freeman, Kagan J, Yarbrough C, Habicht JP. 1972. Is big smart? The relation of growth to cognition. J. Health Soc. Behav.13:219-25.

66. Kuh D, Wadsworth MEJ. 1989. Parental height, childhood environment, and subsequent

height in a national birth cohort. Int. J. Epidemiol. 18:663-68.

67. Lam DA, Miron JA. 1994. Global Patterns of Seasonal Variation in Human Fertility. Ann.

NY Ac. Sci. 209(1):9-28.

68. Lin MJ, Liu E. 2011. Does In utero Exposure to Illness Matter? The 1918 Influenza Epidemic in Taiwan as a Natural Experiment. Work. Pap., Dept. Econ., Univ. Houston.

69. Linnemayr S, Alderman H. 2011. Almost Random: Evaluating a Large-Scale Randomized Nutrition Program in the Presence of Crossover. J. Dev. Econ. 96(1):106-14.

70. León G. 2010. Civil Conflict and Human Capital Accumulation: The Long Term Effects of Political Violence in Peru. BREAD Work. Pap. No. 256.

71. Liu H, Mroz T, Adair L. 2009. Parental Compensatory Behaviors and Early Child Health

Outcomes in Cebu, Philippines. J. Dev. Econ. 90(2):209-30.

72. Loh ES. 1993. The Economic Effects of Physical Appearance. Soc. Sci. Quart. 74:420-38.

73. Lucas A. 2010. Malaria Eradication and Educational Attainment: Evidence from Paraguay and Sri Lanka. Am. Econ. J.:Appl. Econ. 2:46-71.

74. Lumey LH, Stein AD, Susser E. 2011. Prenatal Famine and Adult Health. Annu Rev Publ

Health, 32:237-62.

75. Lundborg P, Nystedt P, Rooth DO. 2009. The Height Premium in Earnings: The Role of

Cognitive Ability and Physical Strength. IZA Work. Pap. 4266.

76. Maccini S, Yang D. 2009. Under the Weather: Health, Schooling, and Economic Consequences of Early-Life Rainfall. Am. Econ. Rev. 99(3):1006-26.

77. MacDonell WR. 1902. On criminal anthropometry and the identification of criminals.

Biometrika 1(2):177–227.

78. Magnusson PKE, Rasmussen F, Gyllensten UB. 2006. Height at Age 18 Years is a Strong

Predictor of Attained Education Later in Life: Cohort Study of Over 950 000 Swedish Men.

Int. J. Epidemiol 35:658-63.

79. Maluccio J, Hoddinott J, Behrman J, Martorell R, Quisumbing A, Stein A. 2009. The

Impact of Nutrition during Early Childhood on Education among Guatemalan Adults.

Econ. J. 119:734-63.

80. Mansour H, Rees DI. 2012. Armed conflict and birthweight: Evidence from the al-Aqsa

Intifada. J. Dev. Econ. 99(1):190-99.

81. Maurer J. 2010. Height, Education and Cognitive Function at Older Ages: International

Evidence from Latin America and the Caribbean. Econ. Hum. Biol. 8(2):168-76.

82. McEniry M, Palloni A. 2010. Early Life Exposures and the Occurrence and Timing of

Heart Disease among the Older Adult Puerto Rican Population. Demography 47(1):23-43.

83. Meng X, Qian N. 2009. The Long Term Consequences of Famine on Survivors: Evidence

from China's Great Famine. NBER Work Pap 14917.

84. Miguel E, Kremer M. 2004. Worms: Identifying Impacts on Education and Health in the

Presence of Treatment Externalities. Econometrica 72(1):159-217.

85. Nelson RE. 2010. Testing the Fetal Origins Hypothesis in a Developing Country: Evidence from the 1918 Influenza Pandemic. Health Econ. 19(10):1181-92.

86. Nilsson JP. 2009. The Long-Term Effects of Early Childhood Lead Exposure: Evidence

from the Phase-out of Leaded Gasoline. Institute for Labour Market Policy Evaluation

(IFAU) Work. Pap.

87. Ozier O. 2011. Exploiting Externalities to Estimate the Long-Term Effects of Early Childhood Deworming. Tech. Rep., The World Bank.

88. Pathania V. 2007. The Long Run Impact of Drought at Birth on Height of Women in Rural India. Work. Pap., UC Berkeley.

89. Pearson K. 1906. On the Relationship of Intelligence to Size and Shape of Head, and to

other Physical and Mental Characters. Biometrika 5:105-46.

90. Persico N, Postlewaite A, Silverman D. 2004. The Effect of Adolescent Experience on

Labor Market Outcomes: The Case of Height. J. Polit. Econ. 112:1019-53.

91. Pietiläinen KH, Kaprio J, Räsänen M, Winter T, Rissanen A, Rose RJ. 2001. Tracking of

Body Size from Birth to Late Adolescence: Contributions of Birth Length, Birth Weight,

Duration of Gestation, Parents' Body Size, and Twinship. Am. J. Epidemiol. 154(1):21-9.

92. Pitt MM, Rosenzweig MR, Hassan N. 2013. Human Capital Investment and the Gender

Division of Labor in a Brawn-Based Economy. Am. Econ. Rev., in press.

93. Porter WT. 1892. The physical basis of precocity and dullness. Transactions of the Academy of Sciences of St. Louis, 6(7):161–81. St Louis: s.n. 1893.

94. Rocha, Rudi and Rodrigo Soares. 2011. Early Life Health and Educational Performance:

Evidence from Rainfall Fluctuations in the Brazilian Northeast. Presented at Health and

Human Capital workshop, ZEW Mannheim, 21/22 July.

95. Rosenzweig M, Zhang J. 2009. Do Population Control Policies Induce More Human Capital Investment? Twins, Birth Weight and China's "One-Child" Policy. The Rev. Econ. Stud.76:1149-74.

96. Richards M, Hardy R, Kuh D, Wadsworth MEJ. 2002. Birthweight, Postnatal Growth and

Cognitive Function in a National U.K. Birth Cohort. Int. J. Epidemiol. 31:342-48.

97. Schick A, Steckel RH. 2010. Height as a Proxy for Cognitive and Non-Cognitive Ability.

NBER Work. Pap. No. 16570.

98. Schultz TP. 2002. Wage Gains Associated with Height as a Form of Health Human Capital. AEA Pap. Proc. 92 (2):349-53.

99. Shay T. 1994. The Level of Living in Japan 1885-1938. In Stature, Living Standards, and

Economic Development, ed. J. Komlos. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

100. Silventoinen K, Kaprio J, Lahelma E. 2000. Genetic and Environmental Contributions to

the Association between Body Height and Educational Attainment: A Study of Adult Finnish

Twins. Behavior Genetics 30(6):477-85.

101. Sørensen HT, Sabroe S, Rothman KJ, Gillman M, Steffensen FH, Fischer P, Serensen TIA. 1999. Birth Weight and Length as Predictors for Adult Height. Am. J. Epidemiol.

149(8):726-29.

102.Sotomayor O. 2012. Fetal and infant origins of diabetes and ill health: Evidence from

Puerto Rico's 1928 and 1932 hurricanes. Econ. Hum. Biol.

103. St. Clair D, Xu M, Wang P, Yu Y, Fang Y, Zhang F, Zheng X, Gu N, Feng G, Sham P, He L. 2005. Rates of Adult Schizophrenia Following Prenatal Exposure to the Chinese Famine of 1959-1961. J. Am. Medical Association 294(5):557-62.

104. Spears, D. 2012. Height and Cognitive Achievement among Indian children. Econ. Hum. Biol. 10(2):210-219.

105. Steckel RH. 1995. Stature and standard of living. J. Econ. Lit. 33:1903–40.

106. Steckel RH. 2008. Biological Measures of the Standard of Living. J. Econ. Perspect.

22(1): 129–52.

107. Strauss J, Thomas D. 1998. Health, Nutrition, and Economic Development. J. Econ. Lit.

36(2): 766-817.

108. Sundet JM, Tambs K, Harris JR, Magnus P, Torjussen TM. 2005. Resolving the Genetic

and Environmental Sources of the Correlation between Height and Intelligence: A Study of

Nearly 2600 Norwegian Male Twin Pairs. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 7:1–5.

109. Tanner JM. 1979. A Concise History of Growth Studies from Buffon to Boas. In Human

Growth, Volume 3: Neurobiology and Nutrition, ed. F Falkner, JM. Tanner, pp. 515-93.

New York: Springer.

110.Thomas D. 1994. Like Father, Like Son; Like Mother, Like Daughter. J. Hum. Resour.

29:950-89.

111.Thomas D, Strauss J. 1997. Health and Wages: Evidence on Men and Women in Urban

Brazil. J. Econometrics. 77(1):159-85.

112.TuvemoT, Jonsson B, Persson I. 1999. Intellectual and Physical Performance and Morbidity in Relation to Height in a Cohort of 18-Year-Old Swedish Conscripts. Horm. Res.

52:186-91.

113.Umana-Aponte M. 2011. Long-term effects of a nutritional shock: the 1980 famine of

Karamoja, Uganda. Tech. Rep., CMPO WP 11/258.

114. UNAIDS, UNICEF, USAID. 2004. Children on the Brink 2004: A Joint Report of New

Orphan Estimates. New York: United Nations Press.

115. Vogl T. 2012. Height, Skills, and Labor Market Outcomes in Mexico. Dept. Econ., Princeton University.

116. Wang X, Ding H, Ryan L, Xu X. 1997. Association between Air Pollution and Low Birth Weight: A Community-Based Study. Environ. Health Perspect. 105(5):514-20

117. Weil DN. 2007. Accounting for the effect of health on economic growth. Q. J. Econ.

122(3): 1265-306

118.Wilson PR. 1968. Perceptual Distortion of Height as a Function of Ascribed Academic

Status. J. Soc. Psychol. 74:97-102

119.World Bank. 2006. Water Quality Management Policy and Institutional Considerations.

World Bank discussion paper.

120.World Health Organization. 2011. Global HIV/AIDS Response: Epidemic Update and

Health Sector Progress Towards Universal Access: Progress Report 2011. Geneva: WHO

Press.

121.Young TJ, French LA. 1996. Height and Perceived Competence of U.S. Presidents. Percept. Motor Skill. 82:1002

 

 



--
To subscribe or unsubscribe from these Child Survival Updates, pls contact kidsurvival@gmail.com. If you unsubscribe, indicate from which E mail address you are receving these updates.
 
When subscribing, write from your most permanent E-mail address, not always that of your current employer.
 
Do not subscribe on behalf of friends or colleagues; forward updates to them for their decision.

Those wishing to read only malaria updates should subscribe at
kidsurvivalmalaria@gmail.com
 

Those wishing to read only vaccination updates should subscribe at kidsurvivalvaccination@gmail.com
 
READER COMMENTS
 
If you have a comment you want posted, send to rdavis@africamail.com

 
WEBPAGE
 
These updates are also available at www.childsurvival.net  

40918755